Transcript for:
Understanding Perception in Psychology

Every rose has its thorn. Only the good die young. Slow and steady wins the race. And what you see is what you get. Except that in  reality, several varieties  of roses do not have thorns; both the good and the bad, on occasion, tragically die young; fast and steady beats slow and steady every time; and what you see is, well... Our perception, or how we order the cacophonous chaos of our environment, is heavily influenced, biased even, by our expectations, experiences, moods, and even cultural norms. And we can be pretty good at fooling ourselves. In the last two lessons, we've learned how we see shapes and colors, hear sounds, and smell and taste the world's chemical concoctions, but our senses  mean little without our  brain's ability to organize and translate that data  into meaningful perceptions.  Without perception, your mother's face is just a combination of shapes. Without the ability to interpret scent, we couldn't differentiate the smell of toast from a grease fire. Our perception is the process that allows us to make meaning out of our senses and experience the world around us. it's what makes life understandable, but also it means that sometimes what you see is not actually what you get. [INTRO MUSIC] So that was awesome, right? Upside-down, I look like me. Right-side-up, I look like some kind of terrifying monster. Your brain isn't used to upside-down faces, so it's basically just doing its best to put the pieces together. But it knows exactly what a right-side-up face should look like, and that is not it. Just one of thousands of examples proving that your brain does all the work of perception, and your eyes really are just feeding raw data. It's important data, but it isn't actually what we see. What we see is the realm of the mind, not the eye. What kind of bird do you see right now? A duck, right? But if I said, "What kind of mammal do you see?" a bunny probably would have popped out at you. Now, you should be seeing both of them popping back and forth, but likely your brain wants to perceive the image related to whichever cue you first heard, or whichever image is more familiar to you. By cueing "mammal" or "bird," I influenced your expectations and you saw what I wanted you to see. Pretty cool! Your expectations are just one factor in your perceptual set: the psychological factors that determine how you perceive your environment. Sometimes, seeing is believing, but perceptual set theory teaches us that believing is also seeing. Context is another factor in your perceptual set. If the duck bunny thing was pictured with easter eggs all around it, you'd think bunny right away -- which is kind of weird, considering that of ducks and bunnies, one is actually much more likely to be near an egg (it's not the bunny). And that's an example of how culture is also an important part of our perceptual set. As much as our perceptions are affected by context and expectations, they're also swayed by our emotions and motivations. People will say a hill is more steep if they're listening to emo by themselves than if they're listening to power pop and walking with a friend. Most of the time, your personal perceptual set leads you to reasonable conclusions, but sets can also be misleading or even harmful. They're  the basis of tons of  entertaining optical illusions. These two tables, for example, are the same size,  but the positions of their  legs make that impossible for you to believe until I  lay them over each other.  And while all the fooling of our visual perception can be fun, it also helps  us understand how it works.  Our minds are given a tremendous amount of information, especially  through the eyes, and we need  to make quick work of it. Turning marks on a paper into words; blobby lumps  into the face of a friend;  seeing depth, color, movement, and contrast; being able to pick out an  object from all the other  clutter around it seems so simple, but we've come to discover that it is quite  complicated. So complicated  that we have a name for it:  form perception. Take a neat little dynamic called the  "figure-ground relationship."  It's how we organize and simplify whatever scene we're looking at into the main objects or figures and the surroundings or ground that they stand out against. The classic "faces or vases" illusion is an example. Is it two faces against a white background, or a vase against a black background?  If you look long enough, you'll  see that the relationship between the object and its surroundings flip back and forth, continually reversing, sometimes white is the figure and black is  the ground. That figure-ground  dynamic, though, is always  there. The concept applies to non-visual fields as  well. Say you're at a party,  holding up the wall and creeping on your crush across the room, trying to  casually listen in on what  they're saying. As the focus  of your attention, that voice becomes the figure, while all  the other voices jabbering  about sports and beer pong  and Sherlock and everything that doesn't have to do with  that one beautiful person  all becomes the ground. Now that your mind has distinguished figure from  ground, it has to perceive  that form as something meaningful. Like for one, that large shape on the couch is a person, and  further, that person isn't  just any person, but the specific unique person of your dreams. One way our minds shuffle all of these stimuli into something coherent is by  following rules of grouping,  like organizing things by  proximity, continuity, or closure. The rule of proximity, for  instance, simply states that  we like to group nearby  figures together. So instead of seeing a random garble of partygoers, we tend to  mentally connect people  standing next to each other. Like, there's the hockey team over there, and the debate team over there, and then you've got the band geeks -- why are all these people at the same party? We're also drawn to organize our world with attention to continuity,  perceiving smooth, continuous  patterns, and often ignoring  broken ones. We also like closure -- and not just after  a breakup. Visually, we want  to fill in gaps to create  whole objects. So here, we see an illusory triangle breaking  the completion of these  circles on the left. But just  add the little lines, close up the circles, and you stop  seeing the triangle. Form  perception is obviously crucial to making sense of the world, or, y'know, a  moderately interesting party.  But imagine trying to navigate the world without depth perception. As you  gaze upon your one true love,  the image hits your retina in  two dimensions. Yet somehow, you're still able to see  the full three-dimensional  gloriousness of their form. You can thank your depth perception for that! Depth perception is what helps us estimate an object's distance and full shape. In this case, a nice shape that is  currently too far away from  you. It is at least partially innate -- even most babies  have it. We're able to perceive  depth by using both binocular  and monocular visual cues. Binocular cues, as the name  gives away, require the use  of both eyes. Because your eyes  are about 2.5 inches apart, your retinas receive  ever-so-slightly different images.  You know, camera one, camera two. So when you're looking with both your eyes, your brain compares the two images to help judge distance. The closer the object, the greater the difference between the two images, also known as the retinal disparity. Retinal disparity is pretty  easy to see, you just hold  your fingers up, and then you look past them, and suddenly you have four instead of two fingers. Because those left and right images vary only slightly, retinal disparity doesn't help much when it comes to judging far-off distances. For that, we look to monocular cues to help us determine the scale and distance of an object. These are things like relative  size and height, linear  perspective, texture gradient, and interposition. Relative size allows you to determine that your crush is not supporting a tiny newborn chihuahua on their shoulder, but  rather, there's a full-grown  chihuahua behind them in the back of the room. In the absence of a chihuahua  (or like object), you  can also judge distances  using your linear perspective. If you've ever made a  perspective drawing in art class,  you'll remember that parallel lines appear to meet as they move into the distance.  Just like the tiled floor,  the sharper the angle of  convergence, and the closer the lines together, the  greater the distance will seem.  And if you've ever looked out at a mountain range or a Bob Ross painting, you'll understand texture gradient as the cue that makes the first ridge appear all rocky and textured, but as your eye follows the ridges into the distance, they become less detailed. And finally,  our interposition, or overlap,  cue tells us when one object, like this oaf here, blocks our view of something else, your crush, we perceive it as being closer. And in this case, especially annoying. So all these perceptual concepts can be demonstrated with a fixed image, but of course, life involves a lot of movement. At least if you're doing it right. We use motion perception to infer speed and direction of a moving object. Like, your brain gauges motion based partly on the idea that shrinking objects are retreating and enlarging objects are approaching. The thing is, your brain is easily tricked when it comes to motion. For instance, large objects appear to move much more slowly than small ones going the same speed. And in addition to organizing things by form, depth, and motion, our perception of the world also requires consistency. Or as psychologists call it, constancy. Perceptual constancy is what allows us to continue to recognize an object, regardless of its distance, viewing angle, motion, or illumination, even as it might appear to change color, size, shape, and brightness, depending on the conditions. Like, we know what a chihuahua looks like, whether it looks like this this, this, or this. In the end, though, your perception  isn't just about funky  optical illusions. It's about how you understand the world and your place in it, both physically and psychologically. Your sensory organs pull in the world's raw data,  which is disassembled into  little bits of information and then reassembled in your brain to form your own model of the world. It's like your senses are just collecting a bunch  of Legos and your brain  can build and rebuild whatever it perceives. A party, your crush, a duck, or a chihuahua. In other words, your brain constructs your perceptions. And if you were correctly constructing your perceptions this lesson, you learned what forms your perceptual set, how form perception works, and the many visual cues that influence your depth perception. Thank you for watching, especially to all of our Subbable subscribers who make this whole channel possible. If you'd like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course Psychology, get a copy of one of our Rorschach prints, and even be animated into an upcoming episode, just go subbable.com/crashcourse. This episode was written by Kathleen Yale,  edited by Blake de Pastino  and myself, and our consultant  is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor is Michael Aranda who is also our sound designer, and the graphics team is Thought Café.